TEACHING EFL/ESL
There has been an increasing awareness of the significance
of integrating literature in EFL/ESL curriculum. Two pedagogically effective
approaches to teaching L1 narrative texts which have been gaining popularity in
EFL/ESL literature are The Story Grammar and The Reader Response. The purpose
of this paper is to present the rationale and conceptualization underlying both
approaches and how they are used in EFL/ESL classrooms.
There has been an increasing awareness of the significance
of integrating literature in EFL/ESL curriculum. The traditional
structurally-based texts, and the newer, integrated, communicative courses
might not be sufficient for the demands of academic classes. On the other hand,
a syllabus that is based, or that draws heavily on authentic stories, provides
a motivating medium for language learning while fostering the development of
the thinking skills that are needed for L2 academic literacy. Literature can
also act as a powerful change agent by developing pupils’ intercultural awareness
while at the same time nurturing empathy, a tolerance for diversity, and
emotional intelligence (Ghosn, 2002, p.172). Emotional intelligence, which is
essential for empathy and tolerance, is the understanding of feelings, both of
one’s own and the others (Goleman, 1995).
There are two pedagogically effective approaches to teaching
L1 narrative texts which have been gaining popularity in EFL/ESL literature:
the “Story Grammar Approach” (SGA) and the “Reader Response Approach” (RRA).
The purpose of this paper is to present the rationale and conceptualization
underlying both approaches and how they are used in EFL/ESL classrooms.
Story Grammar
A recent area of research related to an interactive
conceptualization of reading is story grammar (Ripley and Blair, 1989, p. 209).
Story Grammar is based on the conceptualization that readers should be
consciously aware of text structure. According to this conceptualization,
reading comprehension is an interactive process, an interchange of ideas or a
transaction between the reader and the text (Harris and Hodges, 1995, p. 203).
The reader interacts with the text and relates ideas from the text to prior
experiences to construct meaning. A part of this process requires the reader
understands how the author has organized his ideas, i.e. the text structure.
“Text structure” is a term used to describe the various patterns of how
concepts within text are related. Two important types of text
structure are narrative and expository. Narrative texts tell
a story and are the type usually found in literature selections. Expository
texts provide information and facts and are the type usually found in science
and social studies selections. The types are organized differently, so that
readers must use their comprehension processes differently when reading these
different types of texts.
Research indicates that teaching learners strategies for
focusing on text structure enhances their comprehension and improves their
recall of information presented in text (Taylor and Beach, 1984; Berkowitz,
1986; Wilkinson, 1999). Hence, learners need to be taught how to read different
types of text. They need to learn different strategies for different text types
(Beach and Appleman, 1984, p.116).
Readers can be assumed to have knowledge of discourse
conventions or “textual schemata” that assist in text processing. That is, they
have expectations about what they will encounter when they read stories,
personal letters, research reports, or telegrams (Garner, 1988, p.116). They
use their schemata and clues from the text in varying amounts as they
comprehend (Spiro, 1979). Effective readers use an interactive process that
both relies on their schemata and requires them to obtain information from
text. Even though these two processes occur simultaneously as readers
comprehend, it is the readers’ schemata that provide the structure needed to
associate meaning with text (Anderson and Pearson, 1984).
A story grammar represents the basic structure of a
narrative text. It is the system of rules used for describing the consistent
features found in narrative texts (Mandler, 1984). These rules describe the
story parts, arrangement of the parts, and how the parts are related, i.e. the
internal structure of the story. Story grammars assume that stories have several
unique parts that are conceptually separable, though rarely explicitly
partitioned. These parts are usually identified inferentially by the reader.
There is evidence that such a grammar provides the basis for retrieval of
information from story (Thorndyke, 1977, p. 77).
Although there are several different conceptualizations of
story grammar (e.g. Harris and Hodges, 1995; Leu and Kinzer, 1995; Burns et
al., 1999), all of them include the same basic components (Schmidt and O’Brien,
1986). A simple conceptualization of story grammar is presented by Cooper
(1986, p. 270-271). According to this model, a story may be composed of several
different “episodes”, each consisting of
“a setting, characters, a problem, action and resolution of the
problem”. The setting is the place and
time at which the story occurs. The characters are the people or animals who
carry out the action. The problem is the situation around which an episode is
organized. The action is what happens, or what characters do, as a result of
the problem; it is made up of events that lead to the solution of the problem,
which is called the resolution. A story has a theme: the basic idea about which
the whole story is written, or the lesson the reader learns at the end of the
story. By identifying these elements the reader identifies the story’s
grammar.
A story schema, on the other hand, is the mental
representation that readers have of story parts and
story grammar and a story schema is that the story grammar
deals with the text whereas the story schema deals with what readers have in
their heads about how stories are organized (Amer, 1992).
Direct instruction in story grammar involves helping
learners to recognize the elements of narrative text and use theses elements to
improve their comprehension of the story. Instruction begins with explicitly
presenting the concept of story grammar (setting, characters, problem, action,
resolution and theme). The teacher may use, depending on the learners
linguistic ability, the native language. A strategy teachers may use involves
dividing the story into meaningful episodes and developing comprehension
questions they will ask in guided silent reading and discussion. Such questions
will cause students to focus on the relevant elements in the story. An episode
may consist of one chapter or more. Research has shown that asking questions
that focus on the story line leads to improved learner comprehension of the
story (Beck, 1984; Leu and Kinzer, 1995; Burns et al., 1999). Teachers ask
learners to read, at home, the parts that form an episode and provide them with
guiding questions that bring out the elements of the story grammar. In the
classroom, learners are asked to read silently the parts of the episode which
draw their attention to the story grammar. This is followed by answering the
guiding questions and discussing the structure of the episode. The guiding
questions may be similar to the following (adapted from Cooper, 1986, p.
382-384):
Setting: Where did the story happen? When did the story happen?
Characters: Who
was the story about?
Who were the people in the story? Who was the most
important person in the story?
Problem:
Did the people have a problem? What was the big problem that story was
about?
Action: What did the people do to solve the
problem? What were
the important things that happened in the story?
Resolution: How did
the people solve the problem? How did the story end?
Theme: What lesson could we learn from the
story?
Amer (1992) investigated the effect of story Grammar
instruction on EFL sixth grade students’ comprehension of narrative text.
Results indicated that direct instruction in story grammar seems to help EFL
students abstract the episodic sequence and the metastructure of the story.
Students developed a mental representation of the story, i.e. a story schema,
which helped them focus on main ideas and remove unnecessary details.
Teachers may use visual or graphic representations to
illustrate the story grammar. Visual or graphic representation of text
structure helps learners comprehend and retain textually important information.
Besides, when learners learn how to use and construct visual or graphic
representations, they learn a reading strategy that allows them to identify
what parts of text are important and how the ideas or concepts are related
(Vacca and Vacca, 1999, p. 400). Character maps (figure 1) and story maps
(Willis, 2002) (figure 2) are two common formats used to visually represent key
components of a story. These activities
may be used individually, in pairs, or cooperatively.
Reutzel (1985, p. 401) found story maps to be a good alternative to the traditional
question and discussion session following the reading of a story. They enhance
reading comprehension by helping students to store and retrieve information,
make connections between previous experience and reading materials, identify
relationships among concepts and events, organize specific details, and
understand the message embedded in the text.
Name of the character: --------------
Character trait:
-----------------------
Character trait:
----------------------- Character trait:
----------------------- ……………… Figure (1): Character Map
Title of the Story:
Setting: ………………… ……………
Characters: …………………..………………………… …………………………………………..…
…………………………………………….. …………………………………………….. ……………………………………………..
Problem: ……………….…………………….…………………………
……………………………………………………………….
Major Events: 1- …………………………………………………………. 2-
…………………………………………………………. 3- …………………………………………………………. 4- ………………………………………………………….
5- ………………………………………………………….
Ending/ Resolution: ………………………………………………………………...
67
………………………………………………………………...
Theme: ………………………………………………………………... Figure (2): Story Map
A variation of the story map is the story frame. A story
frame may be used, as a post- reading activity, to test learners’ comprehension
of the story grammar. Story frames focus on the story structure rather than
specific content (Cudd and Roberts, 1987, p. 740). They employ a gap-filling
procedure. Instead of only one word being left out of a sentence, key phrases
or clauses are left out of a paragraph that summarizes the story or highlights
some important aspects of the story. [An example of a story frame (Fowleer,
1982) is presented in figure (3)]. Amer (1992) modified the story frame so that
every missing key sentence or clause is replaced by a question word. Learners
have to answer the questions in the blank lines
In this story, the problem starts when
--------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------.
After that ------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------. Next,
------------------------------------------------------ ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
--------------. Then,
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------.
The problem ends with
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------. Figure (3): Story Frame
It is noteworthy that story maps, character maps and other
concept maps may be generated using computer programs. Two programs written to
produce maps are Inspiration, which is for middle school and older children,
and kidspiration, which is for younger readers. Both programs work well in a
small group or whole class setting when the visual display is presented through
a large screen monitor or projected on a screen. Maps may be printed out for
readers to work independently. Another feature of the two programs is that not
only can the information be viewed as a map, but it can also be viewed as an
outline. This feature helps readers make a connection between the graphic
representation and its outline format. The two programs feature blank formats
so the teacher can create a customized map and templates so the teacher may
utilize a preset model for organizing story information. The templates may be
customized, but they provide a good basis for beginning the creation of a new
map (Slaton, 2001, p. 3). Reader
Response Approach
The Reader Response Approach (RRA) is having a growing
influence on EFL literature classes (Carlisle, 2000, p. 12). The reason is to encourage EFL learners to
study literature for literature’s sake, rather than for the mere attainment of
language skills, which is the popular practice in most EFL classes (Ali, 1994,
p. 289). In these classes, a novel in one hand and a dictionary in the other,
learners plough their way through the
68
pages looking up the new vocabulary until they “understand”
the story. Their focus of attention is not on the experience they have while
reading, but on what facts they can retain for use after reading is over. The
story is not being read as literature but as a piece of information (Carlisle
2000, p. 13). Hence, the teaching of literature is seen as an
information-gathering exercise rather than an aesthetic experience in which the
reader has a response to the event, which involves the organizing of his
thoughts and feelings about the text (Rosenblatt, 1985, p. 40). Benton and Fox (1990, pp. 2-18)
identifies four elements of response to text:
Anticipating/retrospecting: guesses about what is going to happen next,
what events lead to the current situation, and how the book is going to end;
picturing: images that come into the mind’s eye, such as a character’s face or
a scene described in the book; interacting: opinions on a character’s
personality and actions or feelings about events and situations; evaluating :
comments on the skill of the writer.
The RRA is based on Constructivism. It views the reading
process as a transaction between the reader and the text in which the reader,
with his past experiences, beliefs, expectations and assumptions, interacts
with the perspectives in the text, and meaning is determined as the result of
this transaction (Ali,1994, p. 290). Thus, reading, in this approach, is a
reflective and creative process and meaning is self-constructed. The meaning
and structure of the text are not inherent in the print but are invited by the
author and imputed to the text by the reader (Swaffer, 1988, p.124). In other
words, readers are independent makers of meaning. They view text as a
construct. They construct their own meaning. They question the author’s values
against their own values; they differentiate between fiction and reality; they
are able to discuss and evaluate forms of narration and cultural values of the
implied author (Thomson, 1987).
The aim of The Reader
Response Approach is to encourage learners to respond to the text and express
their own ideas, opinions and feelings freely. Thus, learners should realize
that the main concern is not “What they understand” but “how they feel”.
Therefore, the teacher should accept “multiple interpretations” to a text
rather than just one “correct interpretation” (Rosenblatt, 1995). From a
pedagogic perspective, “multiple interpretations” allow for creative and
critical thinking to take place in an atmosphere where there are no threats nor
any compulsion to learn for the “correct” answer or to compete for the “best”
interpretation.
Before using the RRA in classrooms, teachers should first
introduce the RRA. They should explain to students the main ideas and
assumptions underlying the RRA outlined above. Teachers should discuss with
their students the difference between “reading literature” and “reading for
information”. Students should be consciously aware of their contribution to the
text. Several activities and techniques
have been used to implement the RRA in literature classrooms: Reading Logs
(Benton and Fox, 1985; Carlisle, 2000); Response Journal (Sheridan, 1991);
Writing Prompts (Pritchard, 1993); Critical Questioning and Writing (Probst,
1994; Hirvela, 1996); Self-questioning (Davis,1989); Role-play, Drama and
Letter-writing (Elliot, 1990; Baxter, 1999); Rewriting Narratives from Another
Character’s Point of View (Oster, 1989). It is not the purpose of this paper to
present a detailed review of such activities and techniques. Interested readers
can refer to the
69
references. Only two activities are presented as examples:
The Developmental Model of Reader-Response Approach (Figure 4) (Thomson, 1987)
and Reading Logs (Figure 5)( Benton 1992, p. 35; Carlisle, 2000).
Level 1: Literal understanding Students give summaries of
the events of the story. Understanding of the story is at a very superficial
level. Students are merely narrating the information in the text.
Level 2: Empathy Students are involved in the story. They
identify some aspects of the story with their own lives. They also have
imaginative sympathy with one of the characters in the story, and this sympathy
can range from reacting with the character to imagining how the character
feels.
Level 3: Analogy From the readings, students make
connections between the characters and their lives, and from, this, they learn
about their own lives.
Level 4: Interpretation Students reflect on the significance
of events and behaviours in the text. Their reflections lead to generalizations
and evaluations of the characters and theme of the story.
Level 5: Evaluation of fiction Students view text as a
construct. They question the author’s values against their own values; they
differentiate between fiction and reality; they are able to discuss and
evaluate forms of narration and social and cultural values of the implied
author.
Level 6: Recognition Students make a conscious effort to
consider their relationship with the text; they gain implications of
constructedness (aspects of level 5) for their own self-understanding. They
become more aware of their reading process and how they arrive at the meaning
of a text. They are also able to evaluate their relationship with the implied
reader.
Figure (4): Developmental model of a reader-response
approach
While you are reading the book write down all the things
that go on in your head in a “stream of consciousness” style. As you read, you
will be making a record of images, associations, feelings, thoughts, judgments,
etc. You will probably find that this record will contain: Questions that you
ask yourself about characters and events as you read. (Answer these yourself
when you can.)
Memories from your own experience provoked by the
reading.
Guesses about how you think the story will develop, and
why.
70
Reflections on striking moments and ideas in the book.
Comparisons between how you behave and how the characters in
the novel are behaving.
Thoughts and feelings about characters and events.
Comments on how the story being told. For example, any words
or phrases or even whole passages that make an impression on you, or motifs
which you notice the author keeps using.
Connections to other texts, ideas and courses.
An outline of the chapter, no longer than a paragraph.
Please date each entry, and note down the time and place, as
well as the mood you are in while reading. Please note down the page number you
are reading when you make an entry. Please take pleasure and pride in your log.
Please do not try to rewrite the book. Figure (5): Reading logs.
It is noteworthy that some EFL Teacher Education programs
have acknowledged the pedagogic effectiveness of the Reader Response Approach.
Thus, the approach has been integrated in such programs to train EFL/ESL
prospective teachers to use this approach in literature classes (Franklin et
al., 1999). In
conclusion, although the Story Grammar Approach and Reader Response Approach
are based on different theoretical conceptualizations, they should be seen as
complementing each other rather than in opposition to each other. The SG may be
used with beginners and intermediate learners since they may not possess the
linguistic ability to express themselves freely. It may be also used with advanced
learners as an introductory activity to Reader Response. Besides, SG focuses on
the cognitive aspect of learning whereas RR focuses on the affective aspect of
the learner, i.e. his feelings, emotions, free expression, and opinions.
71
References
Ali, S. (1994). ‘The reader-response approach: An
alternative for teaching literature in a second language’. Journal of Reading,
37, (4) 288-296.
Amer, Aly. (1992).
‘The effect of story grammar instruction on EFL students’ comprehension of
narrative text’. Reading in a Foreign Language, 8, (2) 711-720.
Anderson, R. and P.Pearson. (1984). ‘A schema-theoretic view
of basic processes in reading’ in P.Pearson (ed.) Handbook of reading research.
New York: Longman.
Baxter, J. (1999). ‘A message from the old world to the new:
Teaching classic fiction through drama’. English Journal, 89, (2) 120-4.
Beach, R. and D.Appleman. (1984). ‘Reading strategies for
expository and literature text types’ in A. Purves and O. Niles (eds.) Becoming
readers in a complex society. Eighty-third Yearbook of the National Society of
Education. Chicago, Ill.: The University of Chicago Press.
Beck, I. (1984). ‘Developing comprehension: The impact of
the direct reading lesson’ in R. Anderson, J. Osborn, and R.Tierney (eds.)
Learning to read in American schools: Basal readers and content texts.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Associates.
Benton, M. (1992). Secondary Worlds: Literature teaching and
the visual arts. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Benton, M. and Fox, G. (1990). Teaching literature. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Berkowitz, S. J. (1986). ‘Effects of instruction in text
organization on sixth-grade students' memory for expository reading’. Reading
Research Quarterly, 21: 161-191.
Burns, P., Roe, B. and Ross, C. (1999). Teaching reading in
today’s elementary school. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.
Carlisle, A. (2000). ‘Reading logs: an application of
reader-response theory in EFL’. ELT Journal, 54, (1) 12-19.
Cooper, J. (1986). Improving reading comprehension. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin Co.
Cudd, E. and Roberts, L. (1987). ‘Using story frames to
develop reading comprehension in 1st grade classroom’. The Reading Teacher, 41,
(1) 74-9.
Davis, J. (1989). ‘The act of reading in the foreign language:
pedagogical implications of Iser’s reader-response theory’. The Modern Language
Journal, 73, (4) 420- 428. Elliot, R.
(1990). ‘Encouraging reader-response to literature in ESL situations’. ELT
Journal, 44, (3) 191-8.
72
Fowleer, G. (1982). ‘Developing comprehension skills in
primary students through the use of story frames’. The Reading Teacher, 36, (
2).
Franklin, P., D. Laurence, and E. Wells. (Eds.) (1999).
Preparing a nation’s teachers: models for English and foreign language
programs. New York: The Modern Language Association of America.
Garner, R. (1988). Metacognition and reading comprehension.
Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Ghosn, I. (2002). Four good reasons to use literature in
primary school ELT. ELT Journal, 56, (2)
172-179.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam
Books.
Harris, T. L. and Hodges, (Eds.) (1995). The literacy
dictionary: The vocabulary of reading and writing. Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Hirvela, A. (1996). ‘Reader-response theory and ELT’. ELT
Journal, 50, (2) 127-34.
Inspiration 6.0® [Computer software]. (2000). Portland, OR:
Inspiration Software, Inc.
Kidspiration® [Computer software]. (2000). Portland, OR:
Inspiration Software, Inc.
Lehr, F. (1987). ’Story grammar’. The Reading Teacher, 40,
(6) 550-555.
Leu, D. J., and C.K. Kinzer. (1995). Effective reading
instruction. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Amsterdam: New
Holland.
Mandler, J. (1984). Stories, scripts and scenes: Aspects of
schema theory. Hillsdale: N.J: Erlbaum.
Oster, J. (1989). ‘Seeing with different eyes: another view
of literature in the ESL class’. TESOL Quarterly, 23: 85-103.
Pritchard, R. (1993). ‘Developing writing prompts for
reading response and analysis’. English Journal ,82 (3) 24-32.
Probst, E. (1994). ‘Reader-response theory and the English
curriculum’. English Journal, 83, (3) 37-44.
Ripley, W. and T. Blair. (1989). Reading diagnoses and
remediation. Merrill Publishing Company.
73
Rosenblatt, L. (1985). ‘The transactional theory of literary
work: implications for research’. In C.R. Cooper (Ed.) Researching response to
literature and the teaching of literature. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Rosenblatt, L. (1995). Literature as exploration (5th ed.).
New York: The Modern Language Association.
Schmidt, M. and D. O’Brien. (1986). ’Story grammars: Some
cautions about the translation of research into practice’. Reading Research and
Instruction, 26, 1-8.
Sheridan, D. (1991). ‘Changing business as usual: reader
response in the classroom’. College English, 53 (7) 804-814.
Slaton, E. (2001). ‘Building in the Future.’ Paper presented
at National Educational Computing Conference, July 25-27,2001. Chicago,
IL.
Spiro, R. (1979). ‘Etiology of reading comprehension style’.
In Kamil,M. and A. Moe. (Eds.) New Directions in Discourse Processing. Norwood,
N.J.: Ablex.
Swaffar, J. (1988).’Readers, texts and second languages: The
interactive processes’. Modern Language Journal, 72, 123-149.
Taylor, M. and Beach, R.(1984). ‘The effects of text
structure instruction on middle- grade students’ comprehension and production
of expository text’. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 134- 146.
Thomson, I. (1987). Understanding teenager reading.
Melbourne: Methuen & Co.
Thorndyke, P. (1977). ‘Cognitive structures in comprehension
and memory of narrative discourse’. Cognitive psychology, 9, 77-110.
Vacca, R. T. and J.L.Vacca. (1999). Content area reading:
Literacy and learning across the curriculum. NY: Addison Wesley Longman,
Inc.
Willis,C. (2000).
Reading for me; site for Reading Instruction and Strategies for Hammond
Middle School. http:/
acps.k12.va/Hammond/readstrat/Page02Intro.html, 2/7/2002
Wilkinson, L. (1999). ‘An introduction to the explicit
teaching of reading’. In J. Hancock (Ed.) The explicit teaching of reading (p.
112). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
No comments:
Post a Comment