Adapting Brown and
Levinson’s ‘Politeness’ Theory to the Analysis of Casual Conversation
1. Introduction Brown and Levinson’s ‘politeness’ theory was
originally published in 1978. It is a theory that has generated quite a degree
of controversy; although widely acknowledged in the literature, it has also
attracted considerable criticism (eg Matsumoto, 1988; Ide, 1989).
In this paper, I will begin by briefly describing Brown and
Levinson’s ‘politeness’ theory and identifying a type of interaction which their
list of ‘politeness’ strategies does not cover: nongoal-oriented interaction. I
will then go on to analyse a small segment of casual conversation in Japanese
to show how Brown and Levinson’s ‘politeness’ theory can be adapted and applied
in non-goal oriented interaction. This adaptation of their theory to a
different type of interaction has revealed some types of ‘politeness’ phenomena
not described by Brown and Levinson. This paper concludes that ‘politeness’
phenomena can be identified utilising Brown and Levinson’s theory not only in
goal-oriented interaction, but also non-goal-oriented interaction.
2. Brown and Levinson’s ‘politeness’ theory Brown and
Levinson’s work consists of two parts. The first part is their fundamental
theory concerning the nature of ‘politeness’ and how it functions in
interaction. The second part is a list of ‘politeness’ strategies with examples
from three languages: English, Tzeltal, and Tamil.
In the theoretical part of their work, Brown and Levinson
introduce the notion of ‘face’ in order to illustrate ‘politeness’ in the broad
sense. That is to say, all interactants have an interest in maintaining two
types of ‘face’ during interaction: ‘positive face’ and ‘negative face’. Brown
and Levinson define ‘positive face’ as the positive and consistent image people
have of themselves, and their desire for approval. On the other hand, ‘negative
face’ is “the basic claim to territories, personal preserves, and rights to
non-distraction” (p. 61).
Utilising this notion of ‘face’, ‘politeness’ is regarded as
having a dual nature: ‘positive politeness’ and ‘negative politeness’.
‘Positive politeness’ is expressed by satisfying ‘positive face’ in two ways:
1) by indicating similarities amongst interactants; or 2) by expressing an
appreciation of the interlocutor’s self-image. ‘Negative politeness’ can also
be expressed in two ways: 1) by saving the interlocutor’s ‘face’ (either
‘negative’ or ‘positive’) by mitigating face threatening acts (hereafter FTAs),
such as advice-giving and disapproval; or 2) by satisfying ‘negative face’ by
indicating respect for the addressee’s right not to be imposed on. In short,
‘politeness’ is expressed not only to minimise FTAs, but also to satisfy the
interactants’ face regardless of whether an FTA occurs or not.
* I would like to thank Dr Nerida Jarkey, Professor Hugh Clarke, Dr Rod
Gardner, Dr Alan Jones and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments
on this paper and for their encouragement. However, the responsibility for the
content remains with the author.
Proceedings of ALS2k, the 2000 Conference of the Australian
Linguistic Society NORIKO KITAMURA
2
Even though this theoretical part of their work seems to
have the potential to apply to various types of interaction, Brown and
Levinson’s list of ‘politeness’ strategies mainly covers a certain very limited
type of interaction. The examples they give consist mainly of single utterances
which either have or presuppose clear communicative goals, such as asking to
borrow a book or giving advice. Brown and Levinson tend to ignore the fact that
most single utterances are actually just constituents of a larger exchange
between two or more interactants. Firstly, they pay no attention to phenomena
which occur across the entire discourse, such as back-channelling or the
overall sequence of utterances (cf. Schegloff and Sacks, 1973; Scollon and
Scollon, 1981; Usami, 1998). Secondly,
they ignore any interaction, such as simply enjoying a casual
conversation, which does not involve a predetermined goal.
Therefore, this paper discusses how Brown and Levinson’s
theory can be applied to non-goaloriented interaction by utilising naturally
occurring casual conversations as data. The language of these conversations is
Japanese. The analysis shows some examples of ‘politeness’ phenomena which
Brown and Levinson’s list of ‘politeness’ strategies does not cover, but which
have frequently been observed in the data for this study. They can be regarded
as ‘politeness’ strategies according to Brown and Levinson’s general definition
of ‘politeness’: they occur to satisfy the interactants’ ‘face’ regardless of
the occurrence of an FTA, or to minimise an FTA.
3. The data The segment which will be discussed in this
paper is part of a 45-minute-long conversation and is chosen from the
approximately 13 hours of taped data which were collected for a broader
research project. The whole data set consists of face-to-face conversations
between 10 pairs of native speakers of Japanese in their late twenties or
thirties, who regard themselves as close friends.
The data were recorded by the participants in my absence.
The equipment used for the data collection was a cassette tape recorder -- a
small one so that it would not be visually intrusive. I transcribed the data
myself.
4. Participants Before moving on to the analysis of an
excerpt from the data, I would like to introduce the subjects in this segment
of conversation. The two native speaker participants, K and R, are both female
and in their mid-thirties at the time of recording. Both interactants are
working mothers whose jobs involve a lot of responsibility.
5. Summary of the segment of conversation At the beginning
of this segment, principal speaker K begins her story about how recently she has
finally managed to balance things quite well, after being a working mother for
one year. She goes on to talk about the stressful time she had before she
regained her confidence. That is to say, she was used to working as much as she
liked, without thinking of the consequences, but after the arrival of her baby,
she started to modify her work habits. Strangely enough, it was very hard for
her to avoid overworking. The flow of K’s story moves gradually from the issue
of ‘balance’ to K’s stress. In the first part of this segment, K has rather a
fragmented way of talking, and hearer R’s short responses sound very neutral
and function as mere continuers. As the conversation moves on, hearer R tries
to understand what K is saying and to reflect this understanding back to K in
her own words. This patient process of reaching mutual understanding seems to
be the most important thing in this segment of conversation.
Proceedings of ALS2k, the 2000 Conference of the Australian
Linguistic Society NORIKO KITAMURA
3
The segment starts rather quietly, even perhaps with some
slight hesitation indicated by a short silence. However, the exchange gradually
gains liveliness and eventually reaches a chorus of mutual understanding.
Regarding ‘politeness’ phenomena, both ‘positive’ and ‘negative politeness’ can
be seen at the beginning of the segment
while both interactants negotiate their own roles in this part of the
conversation. The more ‘positive politeness’ becomes dominant, the more the
conversation livens up and both participants become actively engaged. The whole
sequence sounds as if one person is making a tentative invitation to dance and
the other, shyly at first, accepts. Gradually they tune in to each other’s
rhythm and eventually start to ‘dance’ cheerfully and enthusiastically in
conversation together.
According to differences in the manner of speaking and
listening of both interactants, this segment of conversation can be divided
into three parts: first, from line 20 to 42; second, from line 43 to 51; and
third, the rest of this segment. Each part will be analysed in turn in the
following section.
6. Analysis of the data 6.1 “Invitation to dance” (Part 1)
: 20 K dekinai ne(..) demo ne:: saikin yappari ne baransu ga
ne:: 21 (..) ichinenme tte iu ka sa:= 22 R =un 23 K (chotto) yappari kyonen wa
sa: 24 (..) 25 R un 26 K kekko: kyonen ichi-nenkan wa= 27 R =un 28 K baransu no
torikata ga sa:= 29 R =un 30 K kekko: muzukashikatta tokoro ga [aru 31 R [a: honto:: 32
K un [kotoshi haitte kara wa ne: 33 R
[a:::: 34 R un 35 K kanari(..) toreru yo: ni natta [(yo: na) 36 R [a:: so: un
un(..) yasumu 37 toki wa yasumu (to) 38 K u:n(..) ato dakara shigoto-suru toki
wa gyaku ni 39 R un 40 K kyonen wa dakara(.) hen ni shigoto-shinai 41 R un 42 K
nante iu no ka na:(..) su to resu ga atta no :
: 20 K [We] can’t, can we?(..) But, recently, y’know, a
balance, y’know:: (..)in the first year, y’know:= 22 R =yeah 23 K well, last
year, [It was] (a bit)... 24 (..) 25 R yeah 26 K the last one year was,
quite...= 27 R =yeah
Proceedings of ALS2k, the 2000 Conference of the Australian
Linguistic Society NORIKO KITAMURA
4
28 K How to find a balance was... y’know... = 29 R =yeah 30
K there were points that were quite difficult. 31 R //aa, really:: 32 K yeah,
from the beginning of this year, y’know: 33 R //a:::: 34 R yeah 35 K ([I]
think) [I] pretty well(..) started to find... 36 R //I see, yeah, yeah.(..)
When one has a rest, 37 one should have a rest. 38 K yeah (..) Then, so when
one works, [it’s] the other way around. 39 R yeah 40 K Last year, I mean,(.)
strangely [I] was [trying] not to work [too hard] 41 R yeah 42 K What can I
say?(..) So [I] was stressed. :
(Transcription Conventions are given after the References.)
The general feature of the first part is that K gradually
gains the status of principal speaker and starts to talk about her situation,
and R accepts this initiation and adopts the role of principal hearer. Both
‘positive’ and ‘negative politeness’ phenomena can be seen in this initial part
of the conversation.
For instance, principal speaker K speaks very slowly and
inserts some short gaps, and hearer R inserts a number of soft minimal
responses which only occasionally overlap K’s speech in lines 22, 25, 27, 29,
31, 33, 34, 39, and 41. These phenomena, which may at first glance, appear
unrelated, seem to be strongly connected and can be regarded as involving both
‘positive’ and ‘negative politeness’.
Starting from the aspect of ‘positive politeness’, the
interactants satisfy each other’s ‘positive face’ by showing their engagement
in this interaction. The principal speaker sounds as if she invites the hearer
into the conversation by creating opportunities for the hearer to insert short
responses at the boundaries of phrasal units (Maynard, 1989). To a certain
extent, one might say that it is the occurrence of the boundary itself which
creates the opportunity for the hearer to respond. However, principal speaker K
enhances this opportunity in this part of the conversation by speaking slowly
and by prolonging the final syllable before the phrasal boundary in many cases.
The hearer, in turn, indicates her engagement in the interaction by taking
advantage of the opportunities created by the speaker and inserting short
responses. As a result of the creation of these opportunities, there are
significantly fewer overlaps in this first part than in later parts of the
conversation.
In addition to this aspect of ‘positive politeness’, the
interactants also satisfy each other’s ‘negative face’ by not imposing on each
other. That is to say, the principal speaker seeks the hearer’s approval to
continue her talk, and the hearer makes soft minimal responses which only
occasionally overlap her interlocutor’s speech, and do not disturb it in any
way.
Another significant feature which can be related to
‘negative politeness’ in this initial part of the conversation is that both
interactants speak rather unclearly in terms of propositional content.
Principal speaker K’s speech is full of ellipses and inversions. For example,
from lines 20 to 35, K does not mention what kind of balance she is talking
about. Moreover, in line 38, K does not complete her sentence. Hearer R also
paraphrases the speaker’s talk with a proverb-like expression in lines 36 and
37: ‘yasumu toki wa yasumu (When one has a rest, one should have a rest)’. This
expression has a broad or generic reference, rather than referring specifically
and
Proceedings of ALS2k, the 2000 Conference of the Australian
Linguistic Society NORIKO KITAMURA
5
directly to K’s situation. In these ways, both interactants
satisfy each others’ ‘negative face’ by utilising unclear expressions, which
can be seen as an attempt not to impose on each other and to give the other
some space to comprehend.
Despite the lack of clarity of their expressions, the
interactants are rapidly approaching mutual understanding. Therefore, these
ellipses and proverb-like expression can also be regarded as indicating their
confidence in the fact that they can understand each other without requiring a
great deal of clarity or detailed description. This can be seen as a form of
‘positive politeness’ which emphasises the fact that the principal speaker and
the hearer share commonality.
6.2 “Let’s dance” (Part 2)
: 43 R A::::(.) A NARUHODO NE:[:? 44 K [un(..) ima wa ne:(.)
konogurai nara 45 daijo:bu da to omotchau to ne[: 46 R [fun fun fun 47 K
ma: i: ya tte omotte ne:[:(..) hora watashi DOCHIRAkatte 48 R [un 49 K iu to sa: cho
tto ko: un kibishi: ka na: tte omou tokoro o: 50 R un 51 K EI YA tte ya
yari nagara yaru taipu da kara sa:= :
: 43 R Aaaa!(.) I see, naturally! 44 K //yeah (..) Now,(.)
‘this much is okay’, y’know, 45 if [I can only just] make myself believe
[it]... 46 R //yup, yup, yup 47 K then, [I] will be okay, y’know.(..) As you
know, I [am] the type 48 R //yeah 49 K -who tends to do things which [I] think
are pretty difficult like this 50 R yeah 51 K - really exerting myself, so
that’s the type [I am], I reckon: = :
The second part of this segment begins with hearer R showing
a great deal of understanding, with the expression ‘A:::: NARUHODO NE::? (Aaaa,
I see, naturally!)’ in line 43. This variation in the hearer’s response can be
seen as an indication of more active involvement. R continues to insert short
responses in lines 46, 48 and 50, while listening to principal speaker K. R’s
short responses sound more rhythmical than they did in the first part. On the
other hand, after having gone through the initial process of tentative
negotiation to assume the role of principal speaker, the pace of K’s speech now
becomes faster and each of her turns becomes longer than in the first part.
The ‘positive politeness’ phenomenon by which the hearer
inserts short responses occurs in a more rhythmical way, and thus starts to
express her more active involvement, rather than just passive attention. The
hearer satisfies the principal speaker’s ‘positive face’ by repeatedly
expressing agreement with her talk. The principal speaker’s faster pace and
longer turns prove that the hearer’s ‘positive politeness’ is functioning well.
The style of the speaker at this point may be likened to the ‘high involvement
style’ described by Tannen (1984). However, whereas Tannen discusses this as a
characteristic style of particular interactants, it appears here as a
Proceedings of ALS2k, the 2000 Conference of the Australian
Linguistic Society NORIKO KITAMURA
6
characteristic of the process of topic negotiation and
establishment in this segment of a particular conversation.
6.3 “Chorus” of mutual understanding (Part 3)
: 52 R =un un [un un un un un un 53 K [shigoto no shikata ga(..) soko o sa:
[EI YA tte 54 R
[un 55 K yaranaide yatteta [kara sa: 56 R [a: a: a: kae
tte [yokunakatta (wake) 57 K [KEKKO:
sutoresu datta no yo 58 R a: a: a:(.) MURI [SHINAIDE OKO: TTE [IU MURI GA ATTA
[(wake)= 59 K [koko
wa [so: so: so: [SO: SO: 60 R =da ne?(.) fu::[::n 61 K SO:
SO: SO::(.)[masa ni sore sore :
: 52 R =yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, 53 K
//The way [I] work is.(..) In that case, [I] wasn’t pushing myself54 R //yeah
55 K -to the limit, so... 56 R //Uh-huh, uh-huh, uh-huh. So actually [it]
wasn’t good [for you]? 57 K //[It] was pretty stressful. 58 R I see.(.) So
[you] were trying not to overdo it and that’s what was such an effort, 59 K
//Here //Right, right, right.. //Right! Right! 60 R wasn’t it?!(.) I see::: 61
K Right! Right! Right!(.) //That’s it, that’s it exactly. :
Moving on to the last part of the segment, the speech of
both interactants overlaps significantly and this part sounds quite noisy. The
absolute dominance of ‘positive politeness’ is the main characteristic of this
part.
Firstly, the hearer starts to show her involvement more
actively than at any stage so far. For example, R indicates her agreement with
rigorous repetition of ‘un (yeah)’ in line 52. She tries to complete K’s talk
(a phenomenon termed ‘co-construction’, eg Lerner, 1991; Hayashi and Mori,
1998) in line 56, saying that it wasn’t good for K. And in lines 58 and 60, she
summarises principal speaker K’s talk, suggesting that K was trying to avoid
overworking and that that was such an effort for K. These phenomena can be seen
as a form of ‘positive politeness’; they satisfy the principal speaker’s
‘positive face’ since the hearer is trying to show her understanding.
Secondly, another ‘positive politeness’ phenomenon can be
observed in this part when the principal speaker emphatically agrees with the
hearer’s summary of her own talk. In lines 59 and 61, principal speaker K says
in a loud excited voice, ‘SO: SO: SO: SO: SO: (Right! Right! Right! Right!
Right!)’, and in line 61, ‘masa ni sore sore (That’s it, that’s it exactly)’.
These comments satisfy hearer R’s ‘positive face’ since they show strong
agreement with R’s summary of what K has said. For this reason, they can be
regarded as further examples of ‘positive politeness’.
Proceedings of ALS2k, the 2000 Conference of the Australian
Linguistic Society NORIKO KITAMURA
7
7. Conclusion The segment of conversation discussed in this
paper involves one of the interactants gradually gaining the status of
principal speaker and continuing to talk about her situation, while showing
respect to the hearer; at the same time, the other interactant gradually
increases her involvement in the conversation, from minimal short responses to
trying to complete or summarise the principal speaker’s talk.
This paper has discussed some ‘politeness’ phenomena which
are not included in Brown and Levinson’s list of ‘politeness’ strategies. These
phenomena are related to the interactants’ manner of speaking and listening and
to the sequence of the exchange in extended interaction. For instance, the
principal speaker introduces a statement gradually, rather than all in one
turn, and the hearer shows his/her involvement by back-channelling or by
summarising the principal speaker’s statement. These phenomena are not related
to goal-oriented interaction and there is no trace of FTAs in the propositional
content of the speech. Nevertheless, they do exhibit certain features which
satisfy the criteria of ‘politeness’ according to Brown and Levinson’s theory, so
they can be regarded as forms of ‘politeness’.
I would like to conclude that Brown and Levinson’s
‘politeness’ theory can be a powerful tool to analyse ‘politeness’ phenomena,
not only in goal-oriented interaction, but also in non-goaloriented interaction
of this nature. However, there is considerable room to expand Brown and
Levinson’s list of ‘politeness’ strategies, such as by focusing on the manner
and sequence of an extended interaction like the casual conversation examined
in this paper.
No comments:
Post a Comment